Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 22
Filtrar
Mais filtros










Base de dados
Intervalo de ano de publicação
1.
Cerebrum ; 20202020.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32395198

RESUMO

Since How to Tame a Fox (and Build a Dog) appeared in 2017, molecular genetics research connected to the silver fox domestication experiment has been published in Nature and Proceedings of the National Academy of Science. Lee Alan Dugatkin writes about his connection to the project, the role of neural crest cells in domesticating foxes, and how dogs, bonobos, and humans fit into the picture.

2.
5.
J Exp Zool B Mol Dev Evol ; 320(7): 407-11, 2013 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23847042

RESUMO

From 1888 to his death in 1921, Russian Prince Peter Kropotkin forced biologists to ask themselves whether natural selection inevitably led to a dog-eat-dog world, or whether pro-social behavior could also be a product of the evolutionary process. In this historical vignette, I focus on Kropotkin's theory of "mutual aid," with emphasis on the role that empathy played in that theory, and the unexpected source--economist Adam Smith's 1759 book, The Theory of Moral Sentiments--of Kropotkin's ideas on empathy in animals.


Assuntos
Evolução Biológica , Empatia , Seleção Genética , Comportamento Social , Animais , Biologia/história , Comportamento Competitivo , História do Século XIX , História do Século XX , Humanos , Rússia (pré-1917)
6.
Cerebrum ; 2013: 1, 2013 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23516663

RESUMO

Many animal species besides humans show evidence of individuality. Knowing how a risk-taker differs from its stay-at-home counterpart could not only help humans live more easily with our fellow creatures, says Lee Dugatkin of the University of Louisville, but also tell us a few things about ourselves and how we got this way.

7.
PLoS One ; 6(5): e19924, 2011.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21603640

RESUMO

Much work in behavioral ecology has shown that animals fight over resources such as food, and that they make strategic decisions about when to engage in such fights. Here, we examine the evolution of one, heretofore unexamined, component of that strategic decision about whether to fight for a resource. We present the results of a computer simulation that examined the evolution of over- or underestimating the value of a resource (food) as a function of an individual's current hunger level. In our model, animals fought for food when they perceived their current food level to be below the mean for the environment. We considered seven strategies for estimating food value: 1) always underestimate food value, 2) always overestimate food value, 3) never over- or underestimate food value, 4) overestimate food value when hungry, 5) underestimate food value when hungry, 6) overestimate food value when relatively satiated, and 7) underestimate food value when relatively satiated. We first competed all seven strategies against each other when they began at approximately equal frequencies. In such a competition, two strategies--"always overestimate food value," and "overestimate food value when hungry"--were very successful. We next competed each of these strategies against the default strategy of "never over- or underestimate," when the default strategy was set at 99% of the population. Again, the strategies of "always overestimate food value" and "overestimate food value when hungry" fared well. Our results suggest that overestimating food value when deciding whether to fight should be favored by natural selection.


Assuntos
Comportamento Apetitivo , Comportamento de Escolha , Alimentos , Modelos Biológicos , Agressão , Animais , Simulação por Computador , Meio Ambiente , Fome/fisiologia , Seleção Genética
8.
Nature ; 471(7339): E1-4; author reply E9-10, 2011 Mar 24.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21430721

RESUMO

Arising from M. A. Nowak, C. E. Tarnita & E. O. Wilson 466, 1057-1062 (2010); Nowak et al. reply. Nowak et al. argue that inclusive fitness theory has been of little value in explaining the natural world, and that it has led to negligible progress in explaining the evolution of eusociality. However, we believe that their arguments are based upon a misunderstanding of evolutionary theory and a misrepresentation of the empirical literature. We will focus our comments on three general issues.


Assuntos
Altruísmo , Evolução Biológica , Aptidão Genética , Modelos Biológicos , Seleção Genética , Animais , Comportamento Cooperativo , Feminino , Teoria dos Jogos , Genética Populacional , Hereditariedade , Humanos , Masculino , Fenótipo , Reprodutibilidade dos Testes , Razão de Masculinidade
9.
PLoS One ; 3(7): e2763, 2008 Jul 23.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18648645

RESUMO

We present the results of an individual agent-based model of antibiotic resistance in bacteria. Our model examines antibiotic resistance when two strategies exist: "producers"--who secrete a substance that breaks down antibiotics--and nonproducers ("cheats") who do not secrete, or carry the machinery associated with secretion. The model allows for populations of up to 10,000, in which bacteria are affected by their nearest neighbors, and we assume cheaters die when there are no producers in their neighborhood. Each of 10,000 slots on our grid (a torus) could be occupied by a producer or a nonproducer, or could (temporarily) be unoccupied. The most surprising and dramatic result we uncovered is that when producers and nonproducers coexist at equilibrium, nonproducers are almost always found on the edges of clusters of producers.


Assuntos
Antibacterianos/farmacologia , Fenômenos Fisiológicos Bacterianos , Farmacorresistência Bacteriana , Algoritmos , Simulação por Computador , Testes de Sensibilidade Microbiana , Modelos Biológicos , Modelos Genéticos , Modelos Teóricos , Seleção Genética , Software , Fatores de Tempo , beta-Lactamases/metabolismo
10.
Biol Lett ; 3(6): 614-6, 2007 Dec 22.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17785260

RESUMO

We examined the impact of winner and loser effects on dominance hierarchy formation when individuals are capable of estimating their opponent's resource holding power (RHP). The accuracy of such estimates was a variable in our simulations, and we considered cases in which all individuals err within the same bounds, as well as cases in which some individuals consistently overestimate, while others consistently underestimate their opponent's fighting RHP. In all cases, we found a clearly defined linear hierarchy. In most simulations, the vast majority of interactions were 'attack-retreats', and the remainder of interactions were almost all 'fights'. Error rates had no effect on the linearity of the hierarchy or the basic attack-retreat nature of interactions, and consistent over and underestimation did not affect the ultimate position of an individual in a hierarchy.


Assuntos
Comportamento Animal , Simulação por Computador , Modelos Biológicos , Predomínio Social , Agressão , Animais , Conflito Psicológico
12.
Naturwissenschaften ; 94(8): 651-6, 2007 Aug.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17354007

RESUMO

Using female mate choice copying as a rudimentary form of cultural transmission, this study provides evidence that social environment during development has a significant effect on the tendency to use culturally acquired information. Groups of newborn guppies (Poecilia reticulata) were raised for 35 days in 1 of 5 "developmental environments". Groups of 15 newborns were raised in pools with no adults (treatment 1), both adult male and female guppies (treatments 2 and 3), only adult females (treatment 4) or only adult males (treatment 5). Mature females raised in treatments 1 and 2, but not treatments 3, 4, and 5, copied the mate choice of others. Treatments 1 and 2 correspond to social structures that guppies experience during their development in the wild. Newborn guppies swim together in shoals (analogous to treatment 1). As they mature, juveniles join schools of adult males and females (analogous to treatments 2). At no time during the normal developmental process are juveniles found with males, but only unreceptive females (as was the case for long periods in treatment 3) or in the presence of adults of only one sex (analogous to treatments 4 and 5). As such, normal developmental environments prime guppies for cultural transmission, while unnatural environments fail to do so.


Assuntos
Comportamento de Escolha , Cultura , Meio Ambiente , Animais , Animais Recém-Nascidos , Feminino , Masculino , Poecilia , Comportamento Sexual Animal , Comportamento Social , Natação
14.
J Theor Biol ; 236(4): 392-6, 2005 Oct 21.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15899500

RESUMO

Inspired by the evolution of antibiotic resistance in bacteria, we have developed a model that examines the evolution of "producers" (who secrete a substance that breaks down antibiotics) and non-producers. In a previous study, we found that frequency-dependent selection could favor an intermediate frequency of producers in a single, large population. Here we develop a metapopulation model that examines the evolution of producers and non-producers. Our results indicate that in a metapopulation with many groups, each of size N, the equilibrial frequency of producers decreases with group size. Even when N is high (e.g. 150 individuals/group), however, a significant frequency of producers is still predicted. We also found that the equilibrial frequency of producers increases as the minimum numbers of producers necessary to provide protection to non-producers increases. Lastly, increasing the benefit/cost ratio (b/c) for producers increases their equilibrial frequency.


Assuntos
Fenômenos Fisiológicos Bacterianos , Evolução Biológica , Farmacorresistência Bacteriana , Ecossistema , Seleção Genética , Adaptação Biológica , Antibacterianos/biossíntese , Modelos Biológicos , Dinâmica Populacional
15.
Proc Biol Sci ; 272(1558): 79-83, 2005 Jan 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15875573

RESUMO

The evolution of group-beneficial traits potentially allows the survival of 'cheaters' that would otherwise be unfit. Here we describe experimental work on group-beneficial traits and the consequences of frequency-dependent selection in the context of bacterial antibiotic resistance. We constructed a 'self-limited antibiotic resistant' (SLAR) strain of Escherichia coli in which a TEM-1 beta-lactamase was anchored to the inner membrane. In pairwise competition experiments between the SLAR strain and ampicillin-sensitive strains, only the SLAR strain survived in the presence of ampicillin. We also constructed a 'shared antibiotic resistant' (SAR) strain in which TEM-1 beta-lactamase protected both the SAR strain and nearby sensitive cells, thus acting as a model for a genetically defined group-beneficial trait. In pairwise competition experiments of the SAR strain against two different sensitive strains of E. coli, we found that the sensitive strains maintained themselves at frequencies of 5-12% in the presence of ampicillin. When the relative cost of the SAR strain was lowered, its equilibrial frequency rose. Sensitive strains also arose from pure cultures of the SAR strain. In these cases, too, the sensitive 'cheaters' were maintained in ampicillin at frequencies comparable to those observed in the previous competitions. These results suggest that traits which benefit other group members can permit survival of genotypes that otherwise would be eliminated by natural selection, and allow the maintenance of greater genetic variation upon which evolution can operate.


Assuntos
Antibiose/fisiologia , Farmacorresistência Bacteriana/fisiologia , Escherichia coli/genética , Variação Genética , Seleção Genética , beta-Lactamases/metabolismo , Ampicilina , Antibiose/genética , Contagem de Colônia Microbiana , Farmacorresistência Bacteriana/genética , Escherichia coli/fisiologia , Genótipo , Plasmídeos/genética , beta-Lactamases/genética
16.
Proc Biol Sci ; 271(1547): 1537-40, 2004 Jul 22.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15306327

RESUMO

Winner and loser effects are defined as an increased probability of winning an aggressive interaction at time T, based on victories at time T-1, T-2, etc., and an increased probability of losing at time T, based on losses at time T-1, T-2, etc., respectively. Prior theoretical work on dominance hierarchy formation has demonstrated that when players are not capable of individual recognition, loser effects always produce a clear top-ranked (alpha) individual, but all other ranks in a group remain unclear; whereas winner effects always produce strict linear hierarchies in which the rank of each individual is clear. Paradoxically, however, when individual recognition--a phenomenon long thought to stabilize hierarchies--is possible, winner and loser effects have no impact on the probability of forming strict linear hierarchies.


Assuntos
Agressão/fisiologia , Evolução Biológica , Modelos Biológicos , Reconhecimento Psicológico/fisiologia , Predomínio Social , Animais , Individualidade
17.
Proc Biol Sci ; 271 Suppl 6: S488-9, 2004 Dec 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15801612

RESUMO

Winner and loser effects have now been documented in a number of species. To our knowledge, experimental work, however, has focused exclusively on pairwise interactions, and not the extent to which winner and loser effects impact hierarchy formation. We report the results of experimentally manipulated winner and loser effects on hierarchy formation in a socially living species, the green swordtail, Xiphophorus helleri. Our results demonstrate that randomly chosen winners in pairwise aggressive contests were more likely to emerge as top-ranked individuals in a hierarchy, whereas randomly chosen losers were more likely to emerge as the lowest-ranking individuals, and that 'winner-neutral-loser' hierarchies were significantly overrepresented.


Assuntos
Agressão , Comportamento Competitivo , Ciprinodontiformes/fisiologia , Hierarquia Social , Animais
18.
Naturwissenschaften ; 90(5): 226-30, 2003 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12743705

RESUMO

Animals assess the fighting ability of conspecifics either by engaging in aggressive interactions or observing contests between others. However, whether individuals assess physical prowess outside the context of aggressive interactions remains unknown. We examined whether male green swordtails ( Xiphophorus helleri) extract information about the fighting ability of solitary individuals via observation and whether acquiring such information elicits behavioral modifications. Contests preceded by mutual visual assessment were significantly shorter than fights where only one or neither of the two individuals was informed in advance. Focal animals initiated aggressive behavior more often against larger opponents only after previewing their adversary, indicating that swordtails can extract information about relative body size from watching solitary conspecifics. When a fighting disadvantage is perceived, observers adopt tactics that increase their probability of winning the contest.


Assuntos
Agressão , Ciprinodontiformes/fisiologia , Animais , Comportamento Animal , Masculino
19.
Behav Processes ; 60(3): 209-214, 2003 Jan 31.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12505174

RESUMO

Bekoff [J. Consci. Stud. 8 (2001) 81] argued that mammalian social play is a useful behavioral phenotype on which to concentrate in order to learn more about the evolution of fairness. Here, we build a game theoretical model designed to formalize some of the ideas laid out by Bekoff, and to examine whether 'fair' strategies can in fact be evolutionarily stable. The models we present examine fairness at two different developmental stages during an individual's ontogeny, and hence we create four strategies-fair at time 1/fair at time 2, not fair at time 1/not fair at time 2, fair at time 1/not fair at time 2, not fair at time 1/fair at time 2. Our results suggest that when considering species where fairness can be expressed during two different developmental stages, acting fairly should be more common than never acting fairly. In addition, when no one strategy was evolutionarily stable, we found that all four strategies we model can coexist at evolutionary equilibrium. Even in the absence of an overwhelming database from which to test our model, the general predictions we make have significant implications for the evolution of fairness.

20.
J Theor Biol ; 220(1): 67-74, 2003 Jan 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12453451

RESUMO

One specific prediction emerging from trait-group models of natural selection is that when individuals possess traits that benefit other group members, natural selection will favor "cheating" (i.e. not possessing the group-beneficial trait) within groups. Cheating is selected within groups because it allows individuals to avoid bearing the relative costs typically associated with group-beneficial traits, but to still reap the benefits associated with the acts of other group members. Selection between groups favors traits that benefit other group members. The relative strength of within- and between-group selection then determines the equilibrium frequency of those who produce group-beneficial traits and those that do not. Here we demonstrate that individual-level selection, that is selection within groups can also produce an intermediate frequency of such group-beneficial traits by frequency-dependent selection. The models we develop are general in nature, but were inspired by the evolution of antibiotic resistance in bacteria. The theory developed here is distinct from prior work that relies on reciprocity or kinship per se to achieve cooperation and altruism among group members.


Assuntos
Evolução Biológica , Comportamento Cooperativo , Seleção Genética , Animais , Modelos Biológicos
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...